2018/09/10 名古屋大学,生理学研究所
本成果は、名古屋大学大学院理学研究科の森郁恵教授らの研究グループと、神経機能素子研究部門の久保義弘教授らの研究グループの共同研究成果であり、9月5日に名古屋大学よりプレスリリースされました。
名古屋大学の青木一郎研究員と森郁恵教授らの研究グループは、久保研究グループと
の共同研究によって、てんかんの原因となる遺伝子異常が、モデル動物である線虫C.
elegansの学習速度に影響することを発見しました。久保研究グループは、電気生理
学的手法による機能解析を担当しました。
この研究成果は、平成30年8月24日付(日本時間19時)英国科学雑誌「Communications Biology」電子版に掲載されました。
※詳細は下記リンクよりご覧ください。
<名古屋大学生命理学専攻HP内ページ>
http://www.bio.nagoya-u.ac.jp/topics/index_279.html
- Ichiro Aoki,
- Michihiro Tateyama,
- Takushi Shimomura,
- Kunio Ihara,
- Yoshihiro Kubo,
- Shunji Nakano &
- Ikue Mori
Communications Biologyvolume 1, Article number: 123 (2018) |
Animals must modify their behavior with appropriate timing to respond to environmental changes. Yet, the molecular and neural mechanisms regulating the timing of behavioral transition remain largely unknown. By performing forward genetics to reveal mechanisms that underlie the plasticity of thermotaxis behavior in C. elegans, we demonstrated that SLO potassium channels and a cyclic nucleotide-gated channel, CNG-3, determine the timing of transition of temperature preference after a shift in cultivation temperature. We further revealed that SLO and CNG-3 channels act in thermosensory neurons and decelerate alteration in the responsiveness of these neurons, which occurs prior to the preference transition after a temperature shift. Our results suggest that regulation of sensory adaptation is a major determinant of latency before animals make decisions to change their behavior.
- Introduction
- Results
- Discussion
- Methods
- Experimental model and subject details
- Behavioral assays
- Forward genetic screen for Slw mutants
- Suppressor screen against slo-2(nj131gf)
- Mapping of nj131 and nj172
- Whole-genome sequencing
- Plasmids
- Knockout of slo-2 by CRISPR/Cas9
- Single-copy insertion by MosSCI
- Imaging analyses
- Electrophysiology
- Quantification and statistical analysis
- Data availability
- Additional information
- References
- Acknowledgements
- Affiliations
- Neuroscience Institute of the Graduate School of Science, Nagoya University, Nagoya, 464-8602, Japan
- Division of Biophysics and Neurobiology, Department of Molecular and Cellular Physiology, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, 444-8585, Japan
- Center for Gene Research, Nagoya University, Nagoya, 464-8602, Japan
- Group of Molecular Neurobiology, Graduate School of Science, Nnagoya University, Nagoya, 464-8602, Japan
- Contributions
- Competing interests
- Corresponding author
- Affiliations
Introduction
One of the central issues in decision-making research is what determines the timing of behavior transition, during which animals learn from a new experience and abandon old knowledge1,2. Appropriate latency before behavior transition is necessary for animals to discriminate long-lasting environmental changes from temporary changes. For instance, animals keep the state of hibernation irrespective of daily change but emerge from it in response to seasonal change3. Nevertheless, if the latency is too long, animals lose opportunities to obtain rewards or fail to avoid dangers. Although regulation of the timing of behavior transition in response to environmental changes is critical for animals, the underlying molecular and neural principles of this process remain largely unknown.
To understand how the timing of behavior transition is determined, we examined the plasticity of the thermotaxis behavior of Caenorhabditis elegans (i.e., association of cultivation temperature with the existence of food and migration toward that temperature on a thermal gradient with no food)4,5 (Fig. 1a). The thermotaxis of C. elegans is essentially regulated by a simple neural circuit4,6 (Fig. 1b). AFD is a major thermosensory neuron in this circuit that increases the intracellular concentration of Ca2+ ([Ca2+]i) in response to a rise in temperature above the past cultivation temperature7,8,9,10,11. Since this cultivation temperature dependency in the AFD dynamic range is well conserved, even when AFD cells are cultured in isolation from the neural network in vitro, AFD cell-autonomously encodes information regarding cultivation temperature8. Genetic analyses have revealed the molecular components involved in temperature sensation in AFD. Three receptor-type guanylyl cyclases, GCY-8, GCY-18, and GCY-23, are specifically localized to the sensory ending of AFD and are thought to act as thermosensors12,13. These guanylyl cyclases synthesize cGMP, which activates cyclic nucleotide-gated (CNG) channels composed of TAX-2 and TAX-414,15, leading to an influx of Ca2+ into AFD.
Thermotaxis behavior is plastic; when cultivation temperature shifts, animals change their temperature preference to the new cultivation temperature over the course of a few hours5 (Fig. 1c, e). Further, AFD neurons that are essential for thermotaxis acclimate to a new temperature by changing the dynamic range of responsive temperature8,16. However, the molecular mechanisms underlying the transition of the temperature preference and the AFD dynamic range are unknown.
In this study, we performed a forward genetic screen for mutants that were slow to change their temperature preference in thermotaxis and demonstrated that a gain-of-function (gf) mutation in the SLO-2 K+channel decelerated the preference transition. The slo-1 gene encodes the other member of the SLO family of K+ channel in C. elegans. Interestingly, the preference transition was accelerated in animals with slo-2 and slo-1 loss-of-function (lf) mutations. Calcium imaging of AFD revealed that SLO K+ channels slowed down AFD adaptation after an upshift in cultivation temperature. Moreover, a forward genetic screen for the suppressors of slo-2gf animals revealed that CNG-3, a subunit of CNG channels, cooperated with SLO-2 to decelerate both AFD adaptation and temperature preference transition.
It was recently reported that gf mutations in a human SLO-2 homolog, Slack/KCNT1, cause early-onset epilepsy17,18. We found that some epilepsy-related mutations potentiated C. elegans SLO-2 in decelerating temperature preference transition. These results imply that the early-onset epilepsy and the latency regulation in C. elegans thermotaxis might have similar molecular and physiological mechanisms.
Results
Isolation of slo-2 gf mutant that is slow to change behavior
To reveal the molecular mechanisms that determine the timing of behavior transition, we performed a forward genetic screen for mutants that were slow to change their temperature preference after a shift in cultivation temperature (Supplementary Fig. 1a). One of the mutants we isolated was nj131. When cultivated first at 17 °C and then re-cultivated at 23 °C for 3 h, wild-type animals migrated to a warm region, suggesting that 3 h of cultivation at a new temperature is sufficient to change their temperature preference. By contrast, a majority of nj131 animals that were cultivated first at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h still migrated to a cold region, showing the old temperature preference (Fig. 1d). Nevertheless, nj131 mutants eventually migrated to a warm region like the wild-type animals approximately 24 h after being transferred from 17 °C to 23 °C (Fig. 1d, e). When nj131 mutants were cultivated constantly at 23 °C, they migrated to the warm region like the wild-type animals (Fig. 1d, f), suggesting that nj131 mutants were not simply cryophilic but slow to change their behavior. Hereafter, we designate this abnormality of nj131 mutants as a “slow-learning” (Slw) phenotype. When nj131mutants were first cultivated at 23 °C and then at 17 °C for 3 h, they migrated to a cold region, as did the wild-type animals (Supplementary Fig. 1c–e). This result suggests that nj131 mutants exhibit the Slw phenotype specifically in response to temperature upshift.
Single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) mapping and whole-genome sequencing revealed that nj131 was a missense mutation in the slo-2gene, which alters histidine residue 159 of the gene product to tyrosine (H159Y) (Supplementary Fig. 1b). We then examined whether slo-2expression can rescue the Slw phenotype in nj131 mutants. Whereas injection of a PCR fragment containing slo-2 genomic DNA into nj131animals did not rescue the Slw phenotype, introduction of mutant or wild-type forms of SLO-2 into wild-type animals phenocopied nj131mutants (Fig. 1g), suggesting that nj131 is a gf allele of the slo-2 gene. When cultivated constantly at 23 °C, both animals expressing the mutant form of SLO-2 and the wild-type animals migrated to a warm region (Supplementary Fig. 1f), suggesting that SLO-2 overexpression did not make animals simply cryophilic but phenocopied the nj131 mutants. Consistent with that nj131 is a gf allele of slo-2, slo-2(nf101) deletion mutants19 (Supplementary Fig. 1b) did not exhibit the Slw phenotype and migrated to a warm region like the wild-type animals when cultivated first at 17 °C then at 23 °C for 3 h (Fig. 1h). If the Slw phenotype of nj131mutants is truly caused by gf mutation of slo-2, knocking out the slo-2gene in nj131 mutant animals should cancel the Slw phenotype. We indeed found that knocking out the slo-2 gene using CRISPR/Cas9 clearly eliminated the expression of the Slw phenotype in nj131 mutants (Fig. 1i), indicating that the Slw phenotype of nj131 mutants was caused by gf mutation of the slo-2 gene.
The slo-2 gene encodes a Ca2+-dependent K+ channel, which consists of an N-terminal transmembrane domain and a large C-terminal intracellular regulatory domain20. The H159Y mutation in nj131 mutants is within the third helix (S3) of transmembrane domain21 (Supplementary Fig. 2). To examine the effects of H159Y mutation on the function of the SLO-2 channel, using the patch clamp method, we recorded whole-cell currents from HEK293T cells expressing either wild-type or H159Y mutant of SLO-2 channels. As reported previously20, depolarization failed to increase outward currents of wild-type and mutant channels when [Ca2+]i was 0 but increased it when [Ca2+]i was >0.6 μM (Fig. 2a, b). At 0.2 μM [Ca2+]i, the H159Y mutant was clearly activated in a membrane potential-dependent manner, while the wild-type was activated only slightly. The conductance–membrane potential (GV) relationship of both wild-type and mutant channels was shifted toward hyperpolarized potentials with an increase in [Ca2+]i (Fig. 2c, d). At the same [Ca2+]i, the GV relationship of the H159Y mutant featured more hyperpolarized potential than the wild-type form, indicating that the H159Y mutant channel is more sensitive to [Ca2+]i and to depolarization (Fig. 2c–e). In terms of activation kinetics, the time constants were smaller (Fig. 2f), and the fraction of instantaneously opening channels was larger (Fig. 2g) for the H159Y mutant channels than the wild-type at the same [Ca2+]i. Therefore, H159Y point mutation increases SLO-2 channel activity, which is consistent with the results of genetic analyses of C. elegansbehavior (Fig. 1g, h).
slo-1; slo-2 double mutants are faster to change preference
Since a gf mutation in slo-2 decelerated the transition of temperature preference, we examined whether loss of slo-2 accelerated the transition. We revealed that the preference transition was not accelerated in slo-2(nf101) deletion mutants (Fig. 3c, e). Then we further examined animals lacking both SLO-2 and SLO-1. SLO-1 is structurally homologous to SLO-2 (Supplementary Fig. 2), and SLO-1 and SLO-2 channels constitute the SLO family of the K+ channel in C. elegans. In slo-1(eg142); slo-2(nf101) double lf mutants, preference transition was accelerated at the time points of 45 min and 1 h after an upshift in cultivation temperature (Fig. 3d, e). Another double lf mutant, slo-1(gk262550); slo-2(ok2214), also showed an accelerated preference transition (Supplementary Fig. 3b and c). These results suggest that endogenous SLO K+ channels function redundantly22 in response to an upshift in cultivation temperature. Both slo-1(lf) and slo-2(lf) single mutants were normal, suggesting that both SLO-1 and SLO-2 act independently. After a downshift in cultivation temperature, slo-1(eg142); slo-2(nf101) double lf mutants changed their preference within the same period as the wild-type animals (Supplementary Fig. 3d), suggesting that SLO K+ channels regulate preference transition specifically when temperature is increased. Although vertebrate homologs of SLO-2 and SLO-1 were shown to interact23, slo-2(nj131gf) mutation decelerated preference transition even in the absence of SLO-1 (Supplementary Fig. 3e), indicating that the gf mutant form of SLO-2 can act independently of SLO-1.
SLO K+ channels act in AFD thermosensory neuron
Since slo-2 is expressed broadly in neurons and muscles (Supplementary Fig. 4a)20, we aimed to identify the cells in which SLO K+ channels act during temperature preference transition. Expression of the H159Y gf form of SLO-2 pan-neuronally or specifically in AFD thermosensory neuron phenocopied slo-2(nj131gf) mutants, while expression in the AWC chemosensory and thermosensory neuron, in interneurons involved in the regulation of thermotaxis, or in body wall muscles did not (Fig. 4a). These results suggest that SLO-2 acts in AFD to decelerate preference transition.
The abilities of the wild-type and H159Y mutant form of SLO-2 to decelerate preference transition were compared by expressing various concentrations of each form in AFD. Injection of a plasmid containing the H159Y form at 1 ng/μl decelerated the preference transition more than injection of the wild-type at 20 ng/μl, suggesting that the H159Y form is far more potent in decelerating the preference transition (Supplementary Fig. 4b and c). These results are consistent with electrophysiological experiments revealing that mutant channels engage in higher levels of activity (Fig. 2c–e).
We next examined whether the accelerated preference transition in slo-1; slo-2 double lf mutants was caused by the loss of SLO channels in AFD. Expression of SLO-1 or SLO-2 in AFD reversed the accelerated preference transition in slo-1; slo-2 mutants (Fig. 4b, c), indicating that loss of SLO-1 and SLO-2 causes accelerated preference transition and that SLO-1 and SLO-2 act in AFD. Together with the results of the cell-specific expression experiments (Fig. 4a), it can be concluded that SLO K+ channels act in AFD to decelerate preference transition.
CNG-3 CNG channel functions with SLO-2
To investigate how SLO K+ channels decelerate preference transition, we performed a genetic screen for the suppressors of slo-2(nj131gf)mutants to isolate molecules that functionally interact with SLO-2 (Supplementary Fig. 5a). Of the mutagenized slo-2(nj131gf) mutant animals, we isolated nj172 mutation, which partially suppressed the Slw phenotype of slo-2(nj131gf) (Fig. 5a, b, d, g). When cultivated at a constant temperature of 17 °C, both nj172; slo-2(nj131gf) double mutants and wild-type animals migrated to a cold region (Fig. 5d, 0 h), suggesting that nj172 does not make animals simply thermophilic but suppresses the Slw phenotype in slo-2(nj131gf) animals.
SNP mapping and subsequent rescue experiments showed that nj172 is an allele of cng-3 (Fig. 5i, Supplementary Fig. 5h and i). The cng-3 gene encodes a subunit of the CNG cation channel. Whole-genome sequencing revealed that nj172 mutants carried a missense mutation in the cng-3 gene, which alters methionine residue 467 to isoleucine (M467I) within a conserved C-terminal cyclic nucleotide-binding domain of the gene product24. When cultivated constantly at 23 °C, nj172; slo-2(nj131gf) animals expressing cng-3 migrated to a warm region similarly to wild-type animals (Supplementary Fig. 5i), suggesting that cng-3 expression does not make animals simply cryophilic but cancels the suppression of slo-2(nj131gf) by cng-3(nj172).
We also characterized cng-3(jh113) deletion mutants. When first cultivated at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h, cng-3(jh113); slo-2(nj131gf)animals migrated toward the warm region on a thermal gradient (Fig. 5f, g), suggesting that cng-3(jh113) deletion mutation also suppresses the Slw phenotype of slo-2(nj131gf) animals. The extent of suppression seemed stronger than that of cng-3(nj172) mutation, suggesting that cng-3(nj172) is a reduction-of-function allele. When cultivated constantly at 17 °C or 23 °C, both cng-3(jh113); slo-2(nj131gf) and cng-3(jh113) mutants did not show a clear preference for a cultivation temperature (Fig. 5e–h)25. The abnormal thermotaxis in cng-3(jh113) mutants cultivated constantly at 17 °C or 23 °C were rescued by expressing CNG-3 in AFD (Supplementary Fig. 6a and b). These results indicate that CNG-3 is necessary for thermotaxis after cultivation under constant temperature but unnecessary for acquisition of a new preference after a shift in cultivation temperature. Other CNG subunits, TAX-2 and TAX-4, are critical for thermosensation; animals lacking either TAX-2 or TAX-4 were completely athermotactic, regardless of whether cultivation temperature was shifted (Supplementary Fig. 6c).
It was reported that CNG-3 is expressed in sensory neurons, including AFD26,27. To determine the site(s) at which CNG-3 acts, we cell-specifically expressed CNG-3 in cng-3(nj172); slo-2(nj131gf) mutants. Expression of CNG-3 in AFD but not in AWC canceled suppression by cng-3(nj172) (Fig. 5j), suggesting that, like SLO channels, CNG-3 functions in AFD (Fig. 4).
SLO and CNG channels slowed down AFD adaptation
Since SLO-2 was shown to act in the AFD thermosensory neuron to decelerate temperature preference transition, we next examined whether SLO-2 acts upstream of Ca2+ responses in AFD using Ca2+imaging. AFD responds to rises in temperature above the past cultivation temperature7,8. In wild-type animals cultivated constantly at 17 °C, AFD increased the Ca2+ level at approximately 15 °C in response to a temperature increase from 14 °C to 24 °C (Fig. 6a, g). In wild-type animals, the temperature leading to onset of AFD responses when cultivated constantly at 23 °C was around 23 °C, but the onset temperature of AFD response when cultivated first at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h was around 21 °C. Although we did not detect any obvious abnormality in the onset temperature of slo-2(nj131gf) animals cultivated constantly at 17 °C or at 23 °C, the onset temperature in slo-2(nj131gf)animals cultivated first at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h was lower than in wild-type animals (Fig. 6b, g). When cultivated longer at 23 °C, the onset temperature for slo-2(nj131gf) animals gradually became comparable to that for wild-type animals (Fig. 6h). These results suggest that slo-2(nj131gf) mutation slowed down transition of the AFD dynamic range (i.e., AFD adaptation). AFD adaptation preceded behavioral transition in both wild-type animals and slo-2(nj131gf) mutants (Figs. 1eand 6h). These results indicate that a mechanism is acting either downstream of the Ca2+ influx in AFD or in downstream neural circuits, which may explain that the transition of the AFD dynamic range is transformed onto the behavior with delay. Moreover, expression of the gf form of SLO-2 specifically in AFD neurons lowered the onset temperature for animals cultivated first at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h (Fig. 6e, g), suggesting that enhanced SLO-2 currents in AFD slow down AFD adaptation cell-autonomously.
Since the Slw phenotype in slo-2(nj131gf) mutants was suppressed by cng-3 mutation, we next examined whether the slowed AFD adaptation in slo-2(nj131gf) mutants after an upshift in cultivation temperature was also suppressed by cng-3 mutation. When cultivated first at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h, cng-3(jh113) suppressed the lowered onset temperature of AFD in slo-2(nj131gf) animals to a level comparable to that of wild-type animals (Fig. 6d, g). Moreover, AFD-specific expression of CNG-3 in cng-3(jh113); slo-2(nj131gf) double mutants canceled the suppression of decelerated adaptation by cng-3 lf mutation (Fig. 6f, g). These results suggest that CNG-3 functionally interacts with SLO-2 in AFD to slow down AFD adaptation and thereby to decelerate behavioral transition. Although cng-3(jh113) mutants showed aberrant thermotaxis behavior when cultivated constantly at 17 °C or at 23 °C (Fig. 5e), AFD in cng-3(jh113) mutants responded to a temperature increase at around the cultivation temperature, like the wild-type animals, except for a slower decrease in the level of Ca2+ (Fig. 6c). This is in a sharp contrast to tax-2or tax-4 mutants, in which AFD is totally inactive8,9.
We next examined how endogenous SLO channels affect the timing of changes in the AFD dynamic range. The dynamic range of AFD changes within a few hours after a shift in cultivation temperature16,28, whereas AFD is irresponsive to an increase in temperature beyond the temperature 5–6 °C higher than the cultivation temperature8. Thus animals cultivated at 17 °C were investigated when their AFD regained the responsiveness after an upshift in cultivation temperature. To do so, we used a temperature program that produced short-term temperature oscillations (20 s, 0.05 Hz) around 17 °C followed by a shift to 23 °C, which mimicked an upshift in cultivation temperature, and long-term oscillations around 23 °C (Fig. 7a–e). In wild-type animals, AFD exhibited phase-locked responses to oscillations around 17 °C, a large response to a temperature upshift and gradual Ca2+ decrease, and then faintly regained phase-locked responses to oscillations around 23 °C 20 min after the upshift (Fig. 7a). In contrast, in slo-2(nj131gf) animals, AFD showed a sharp decrease in Ca2+ level and profound silencing after the temperature upshift and did not regain the ability for phase-locked response in 20 min (Fig. 7b). On the other hand, in slo-1; slo-2 double lf mutants, AFD showed clear phase-locked responses to oscillations from an earlier time point than wild-type animals (Fig. 7e). Use of the Fourier transform to examine the AFD responses further supported these observations (Fig. 7f–j). First, Fourier transform of the sequence of temperature revealed a clear peak at 0.05 Hz. The peak at 0.05 Hz in the AFD responses of slo-1; slo-2 double lf mutants was much more explicit than that of wild-type animals (Fig. 7f, j). In time courses of the Fourier transform, the Fourier components of AFD response other than those at or near 0.05 Hz disappeared earlier in slo-1; slo-2 double lf mutants than in wild-type animals (Fig. 7k, o). Taken together, these results further support that SLO K+ channels slow down AFD adaptation after an upshift in cultivation temperature.
Epilepsy-related mutations potentiate SLO-2
Early-onset forms of epilepsy such as malignant migrating partial seizures of infancy and autosomal-dominant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy can be caused by gf mutations in the slo-2 human homolog Slack/KCNT117,18, which encodes a Na+-gated K+ channel mainly expressed in the brains29. Epilepsy-related mutant forms of the Slack channel feature enhanced channel activity17,30,31,32. Analogously, the H159Y mutant form of C. elegans SLO-2 isolated in this study featured higher channel activity (Fig. 2), suggesting that both human epilepsy and decelerated temperature preference transition in thermotaxis are caused by excess K+ efflux through SLO-2 channels. Therefore, we tested whether introduction of epilepsy-related mutations in C. elegansSLO-2 (Supplementary Figs. 2 and 7) could phenocopy the gf nature of H159Y for decelerating the preference transition in thermotaxis.
We introduced each of six epilepsy-related mutations into C. elegans slo-2 DNA and injected each DNA into slo-2(nf101) deletion mutants. Animals expressing wild-type or mutant SLO-2 in AFD were cultivated first at 17 °C, then at 23 °C for 3 h, and then were subjected to thermotaxis assay. Expression of two of six epilepsy-related mutant forms of SLO-2 more greatly decelerated preference transition than did expression of wild-type SLO-2 (Fig. 8a, b). These results imply that, although slow preference transition in C. elegans thermotaxis and early-onset epilepsy in humans are macroscopically divergent, both events result from common cellular processes initiated by a gf mutation in the SLO-2 channel. Excessive activity of SLO channels and resulting excessive repolarization after depolarization either in AFD thermosensory neurons in C. elegans or in neurons in the human brains seem to cause the respective event. These results also indicated that these two mutations potentiate SLO-2/Slack channels, regardless of their differences such as gating by Ca2+ or Na+. We also found that cng-3mutation suppressed deceleration of preference transition caused by the expression of SLO-2(R376Q) in AFD (Fig. 8c), implying that inhibition of CNG channels might attenuate epilepsy symptoms. It is of clinical interest to examine whether Slack(gf) rodent models show epilepsy-related phenotypes and whether CNG inhibitors attenuate those phenotypes.
Discussion
In this study, we demonstrated that, upon an upshift in cultivation temperature, SLO K+ channels function with the CNG-3 channel in the AFD thermosensory neuron to slow down the transition of the AFD dynamic range and thereby generate latency for transition of the temperature preference. An upshift in the temperature results in a large influx of Ca2+ into AFD, and consequently K+ efflux through SLO channels repolarizes the membrane potential and decreases [Ca2+]i, which antagonize premature AFD adaptation (Fig. 8d). Although the expression levels of AFD-specific guanylyl cyclases are shown to increase when cultivation temperature is increased28, this induction was unaffected by slo-2(nj131gf) mutation (Supplementary Fig. 8a–c). We also found a molecular and physiological link between early-onset epilepsy and preference transition in thermotaxis, providing a model system that can be used to understand the pathogenesis of this type of epilepsy and to screen for drugs.
Although CNG channels are generally thought to depolarize the membrane potential24, our results implied that CNG-3 acts with SLO K+channels to hyperpolarize AFD (Fig. 5). Considering that SLO-1 and SLO-2 of C. elegans are both gated by Ca2+33 and that CNG channels are generally permeable to Ca2+24, it is reasonable to hypothesize that SLO and CNG channels are co-localized in a limited region in which local Ca2+ influx through CNG channels, including CNG-3, activates SLO K+channels.
While the onset temperature of AFD in slo-2(nj131gf) animals cultivated at 17 °C was comparable with that in wild-type animals, the Ca2+ level decreased sharply beneath the base line in slo-2(nj131gf) animals (Fig. 6b). This result is consistent with the important roles of SLO-2 in post-excitatory repolarization of the membrane potential in body wall muscles29,34,35 and in motor neurons36 in C. elegans and in mammalian neurons37,38. This altered AFD response accompanied with the diffused distribution during thermotaxis behavior of animals cultivated at 17 °C in both slo-2(nj131gf) animals (Figs. 6b, 1d) and animals with AFD-specific SLO-2 expression (Fig. 6e and Supplementary Fig. 7f, 0h and 7i). In addition, both the altered AFD response and the diffused distribution of the animals were suppressed by cng-3 mutation (Figs. 6d and 5d). Thus the sharp decrease in Ca2+ in AFD may result in insufficient navigation during thermotaxis.
Although a number of studies have focused on alteration of the relationships between unconditional stimuli (US) and conditional stimuli (CS)39, our study focused on CS transition. C. elegans associate CS such as temperature or chemicals with preferable US such as the existence of food, and the conditioned animals are attracted to the CS. Similarly, they can also associate CS with unpreferable US and no longer be attracted to the CS or even avoid it. For instance, the role of the insulin–phosphoinositide-3 kinase–Akt pathway was intensively studied during association between CS with starvation as a negative US40,41,42. The mechanisms underlying decay of the US–CS association have also been reported43,44,45. In this study, we performed a forward genetic screen that focused on CS transition and newly identified molecules that do not have any reported roles in learning. dgk-3 mutants were previously reported to be slow to change temperature preference in thermotaxis after cultivation temperature was shifted16. However, dgk-3mutants in our experimental set-up were simply cryophilic. In this study, our results suggested that the SLO and CNG channels act together to modulate transition of the dynamic range of a sensory neuron that can encode the CS.
Unveiling the mechanisms underlying short- and long-term memory (STM and LTM, respectively) and consolidation of STM into LTM is an important issue in neuroscience. The role of the cAMP–protein kinase A–CREB pathway in consolidation of Pavlovian conditioning is conserved among Aplysia, flies, and mammals. Our results may provide a foundation for further genetic research to reveal the mechanisms underlying consolidation. Since the original report on thermotaxis5, it has been known that temperature preference reaches a local maximum 2–3 h after a shift in cultivation temperature, is slightly weakened, and then reaches a plateau (Fig. 1e). We found that slo-2(nj131gf) animals were slow to change their behavior, whereas cng-3(jh113) animals could transiently acquire an ability to perform thermotaxis immediately after a shift in cultivation temperature but gradually lost the ability when cultivated for longer at the new temperature. One possibility is that two different components, one of which is fast but transient and the other of which is delayed but persistent, might help shape the transition of temperature preference (Supplementary Fig. 8d). Our results suggested that SLO and CNG channels suppress the fast component and that the delayed component is dependent on CNG-3 but not on SLO channels, since slo-1; slo-2 double lf mutants exhibited normal thermotaxis when cultivated for a long time at a certain temperature. Revealing the mechanisms by which each component is regulated, through screening for suppressors for cng-3 mutants or analyzing animals in a transient state of consolidation, might lead to a comprehensive understanding of consolidation of STM into LTM.
Vertebrate CNG channels, which are essential for the functions of sensory neurons such as rod and cone photoreceptors and olfactory receptor neurons, are also expressed in the central nervous system and play versatile roles46. CNG channels with different subunit composition have different properties, such as sensitivity to cNMP and ion permeability, and function in different cell types24. The results of this study and others provide clues that improve our understanding of how CNG channels with distinct subunit compositions contribute to different cellular processes. Among the CNG channels in C. elegans, TAX-2 and TAX-4 are essential for thermotaxis as well as for chemotaxis14,15, but CNG-3 seems to play an auxiliary role47. Although an α subunit TAX-4 can form a functional homo-tetramer that is far more sensitive to cGMP than the heteromer consisting of TAX-4 and a β subunit TAX-248 and TAX-2 cannot form a functional homomer by itself, TAX-2 is essential for thermosensation and chemosensation, indicating that heteromer-specific functions exist. CNG-3 was suggested to form a hetero-tetramer with TAX-4 and TAX-2 in AWC47, implying that a similar heteromer also forms in AFD. Our results suggest that CNG-3 is necessary for steady, but not for transient, thermotaxis. Thus investigating how CNG channels with different compositions function in specific behavioral or cellular contexts in combination with the analysis of their electrophysiological properties would provide further understanding of how CNG channels function in the nervous system.
This study, focusing on a transient state of decision making, not only provided scientific insights into the mechanisms that determine the timing of decision making but also revealed possible clinical applications that may contribute to the treatment of early-onset epilepsy. Our results suggested that the preference transition in thermotaxis might be useful as a model system for early-onset epilepsy caused by Slack/KCNT1 mutations. Given that quinidine, which blocks the Slack channel49,50, was not effective in every case of Slack/KCNT1-related epilepsy51,52,53 and that quinidine blocks other ion channels and may cause cardiac toxicity50, screening for drugs that specifically affect Slack using C. elegans would be valuable. Since Slack is expressed in cardiomyocytes29, identification of molecules that functionally interact with Slack only in neurons by screening suppressors for slo-2(nj131gf)animals would identify better drug targets.
Methods
Experimental model and subject details
C. elegans strains were maintained as described54. Briefly, animals were cultivated on nematode growth medium (NGM) plates seeded with an Escherichia coli OP50 strain (Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (CGC), Twin Cities, MN, USA). N2 (Bristol) was used as the wild-type strain unless otherwise indicated. Transgenic lines were generated as described55. Briefly, plasmid DNA or PCR fragments were directly injected into the hermaphrodite gonad. Strains used in this study are listed in Supplementary Table 2.
Behavioral assays
Population thermotaxis assays were performed as described previously56. Briefly, 50–250 animals cultivated at 17 °C or 23 °C or subjected to a shift in cultivation temperature were placed at the center of the assay plates without food and with a temperature gradient of 17–23 °C. The animals were allowed to freely move for 60 min. The assay plate was divided into eight sections along the temperature gradient, and the number of adult animals in each section was scored. The ratio of animals in each section was plotted on histograms. Thermotaxis indices were calculated as shown below:
where Ni: number of animals in each section i (i = 1–8) and N: total number of animals on the test plate.
Forward genetic screen for Slw mutants
For mutagenesis, wild-type animals were treated with 50 mM ethyl methanesulfonate (EMS; Nacalai, Kyoto, Japan) for 4 h at room temperature. The F1 generation of the mutagenized animals was cultivated at 17 °C for 5 days and was allowed to self-fertilize and give rise to the F2 generation. F2 animals were cultivated at 23 °C for 3 h and then put on a thermal gradient for thermotaxis assay. Most animals migrated to the warm region of the thermal gradient. Animals that still migrated to the cold region and thus were putatively slow to change behavior, were isolated. To discriminate between animals that are slow to change behavior and those that constitutively prefer cold temperature, the strains isolated above were cultivated constantly at 23 °C and then subjected to thermotaxis assay. Animals that migrated to the cold region when cultivated at 17 °C and then at 23 °C for 3 h but migrated to the warm region when cultivated constantly at 23 °C were considered to be slow to change behavior.
Suppressor screen against slo-2(nj131gf)
As described above, slo-2(nj131gf) animals were mutagenized with EMS. Mutagenized animals were cultivated at 17 °C for 5 days and at 23 °C for 3 h and then were subjected to thermotaxis assay as described above. Most animals still migrated to the cold region of the thermal gradient. Animals that migrated to the warm region like the wild-type animals were isolated.
Mapping of nj131 and nj172
We crossed nj131 animals with a wild-type polymorphic CB4858 strain57. Since the Slw phenotype of nj131 was inherited semidominantly, we isolated F2 animals that exhibited Slw or wild-type phenotypes and identified crossover sites as described58. We mapped nj131 to a 1.3-Mb interval between nucleotides 11,136,567 and 12,419,537 on linkage group (LG) X.
To map nj172, we first crossed slo-2(nj131gf) animals with CB4858 four times to obtain a strain in which the slo-2(nj131gf) allele exists on the CB4858 genome (IK2092). We then crossed nj172; slo-2(nj131gf) with IK2092, isolated F2 animals in which the Slw phenotype was suppressed and identified crossover sites. We mapped nj172 to a 1.1-Mb interval between nucleotides 8,723,437 and 9,847,895 on LG IV.
Whole-genome sequencing
Genomic DNA was purified with the Puregene Core Kit A (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). The genome was sequenced with MiSeq (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA), and the sequence was analyzed with CLC Genomics Workbench (CLC Bio, Aarhus, Denmark).
Plasmids
pOX nSlo2 was a gift from Larry Salkoff (Addgene plasmid #16202)20. A DNA clone including a part of cng-3 cDNA (yk348a9) was provided by Yuji Kohara. To generate plasmids to cell-specifically express slo-2 or cng-3, we fused promoter sequences of gcy-8, ceh-36, ttx-3, lin-11, glr-3, or myo-3; the cDNA of slo-2 or cng-3; and the unc-54 3’UTR sequence by MultiSite Gateway Technology (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). To generate plasmids to express SLO-2b in HEK293T cells, slo-2bcDNA was inserted into a pCAG vector. Details regarding the plasmid constructs can be obtained from the authors. Peft-3::Cas9-SV40 NLS::tbb-2 3′-UTR (Addgene plasmid #46168) and PU6::unc-119_sgRNA(#46169) were gifts from John Calarco59. The unc-119 single-guide RNA (sgRNA) sequence in PU6::unc-119_sgRNA was replaced with the sequences of sgRNAs for unc-22, slo-2 #1, and slo-2 #4, which were GAACCCGTTGCCGAATACAC(AGG)60, GAAATACCTGGTTTTCGGGG(TGG), and GCACAATGCACGATGCAAGG(CGG), respectively. Erik Jorgensen gifted pCFJ150 pDESTttTi5605[R4-R3] (Addgene plasmid #19329), pCFJ601 Peft-3::Mos1 transposase (#348874), pMA122 Phsp::peel-1 (#348873), pCFJ90 myo-2p::mCherry (#19327), pCFJ104 myo-3::mCherry (#19328), and pGH8 rab-3p::mCherry (#19359)61,62. A plasmid containing slo-1cDNA was a gift from Jonathan T. Pierce63. The gcy-8 promoter sequence, slo-1 cDNA, and unc-54 3’UTR sequence were fused into pCFJ150 to make pIA109. Plasmids used in this study are listed in Supplementary Table 3.
Knockout of slo-2 by CRISPR/Cas9
The slo-2 gene was knocked out by CRISPR/Cas9 as described60,64. First, slo-2(nj131gf) animals were injected with Peft-3::Cas9-SV40 NLS::tbb-2 3′-UTR, pRF4 rol-6(su1006, gf), pSN680 unc-22 sgRNA, pIA009 slo-2 sgRNA #1, and pIA054 slo-2 sgRNA #4. Then unc-22/+ and unc-22/unc-22 F1 twitcher animals were subjected to genotyping for the slo-2 gene locus. Although deletion between the sites of sgRNA #1 and #4 was not detected, small indels around the sgRNA #1 site were detected by polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis following PCR that amplified 60 bp region including the site of sgRNA#1 with primers TCGTGATTTCTTGGAAGAATTCT and tggtgtgttactgaaatacCTGG. A strain that homozygously carried a small deletion at the slo-2 locus was isolated from the progeny of a unc-22/+ animal with a small deletion. Sequencing of the genomic slo-2 locus revealed that, in this strain, two nucleotides in the seventh exon are deleted, causing a frame shift after His344. No indel was found adjacent to the site of sgRNA #4. Then unc-22 mutation was knocked out by selecting non-twitcher animals.
Single-copy insertion by MosSCI
A single copy of gcy-8p::slo-1a(+) or gcy-8p::slo-2b(+) was inserted into a genome as described62. First, ttTi5605; unc-119(ed3) animals derived from the EG6699 strain61, which feature uncoordinated (Unc) locomotion, were injected with pIA109 or pIA110 in addition to pCFJ601, pMA122, pGH8, pCFJ90, and pCFJ104. Then NGM plates with non-Unc animals were heat-shocked at 42 °C for 2 h. Non-Unc animals without mCherry fluorescence were selected and subjected to genotyping. PCR was performed to confirm insertion of a full-length transgene.
Imaging analyses
Calcium imaging was performed as described elsewhere8,65. A single adult animal that expressed the genetically encoded calcium indicators R-CaMP266 in AFD and GCaMP367 in AIY or an animal that expressed GCaMP3 and tagRFP in AFD was placed on a 10% agar pad on a cover slip with 0.1 µm polystyrene beads (Polysciences, Warrington, PA, USA) and covered with another cover slip for immobilization68. The immobilized animals were placed on a Peltier-based temperature controller (Tokai Hit, Fujinomiya, Japan) on a BX61WI microscope (Olympus, Tokyo, Japan). Red and green fluorescence was separated by the Dual-View optics system (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA, USA), and images were captured by an ImageEM EM-CCD camera (C9100-13, Hamamatsu Photonics, Japan) at a frame rate of 1 Hz. Excitation pulses were generated by a SPECTRA light engine (Lumencor, Beaverton, OR, USA). Fluorescence intensities were measured using the MetaMorph imaging system (Molecular Devices).
Expression of SLO-2::mCherry was observed with a BX53 upright microscope (Olympus).
Electrophysiology
HEK293T cells were transfected with plasmids encoding yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) and either the wild-type or H159Y mutant form of SLO-2b with Lipofectamine2000 (Invitrogen). The macroscopic current in HEK293T cells expressing YFP in a whole-cell patch clamp configuration was recorded using the Axopatch 200B amplifiers, Digidata 1322 A, and pClamp 9 software (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA, USA) at room temperature, as previously described69. The bath solution contained (in mM) 140 NaCl, 4 KCl, 10 HEPES, 1 CaCl2, and 0.3 MgCl2 (pH = 7.4). Pipette resistances ranged from 1 to 3 MΩ when the glass pipette was filled with the pipette solution, which contained (in mM) 130 KCl, 10 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 5 K2ATP, 4 MgCl2, 3 EGTA, 0.3 GTP, and various concentration of CaCl2 (pH = 7.3). To achieve 0, 0.2, 0.6, 2, 6, and 20 µM of free [Ca2+]i, 1, 1.96, 2.55, 2.86, 2.99, and 3.10 mM of CaCl2, respectively, were added to the pipette solution, according to the Ca-Mg-ATP-EGTA Calculator v1.0 using constants from the NIST database #46 v8 (http://maxchelator.stanford.edu/CaMgATPEGTA-NIST.htm). After rupture of the sealed membrane, cells were held at -60 mV for at least 3 min to dialyze the pipette solution into the cell, and then depolarizing step pulses (10 mV increment for 200 ms) were applied. Immediately after application of the step pulse, cells were held at 0 mV (50 ms) for tail current analysis. The current amplitudes were measured at the end of the step pulses and normalized by the membrane capacitance of each cell. Then the current densities were averaged and plotted as a function of the membrane potentials. To analyze the voltage dependence of the channels, the tail current amplitudes at the beginning of 0 mV were measured and normalized by the maximal amplitude at the most depolarized potential (G/Gmax). G/Gmax values were plotted against the membrane potential and fitted to a Boltzmann function to estimate the membrane potential of half-maximal activation (V1/2) in each cell. The activation kinetics of the channels was analyzed by fitting the traces of outward current to double exponential functions. Several traces that were not fitted to the double exponential functions were eliminated from the kinetic analyses.
Quantification and statistical analysis
The error bars in histograms and line charts indicate the standard error of mean (SEM). In the boxplots, the bottom and top of boxes represent the first and third quartiles, and the band inside the box represents the median. The ends of the lower and upper whiskers represent the lowest datum still within the 1.5 interquartile range (IQR), which is equal to the difference between the third and first quartiles, of the lower quartile, and the highest datum still within the 1.5 IQR of the upper quartile, respectively. For multiple-comparison tests, one-way analyses of variance were performed, followed by Tukey–Kramer or Dunnett tests, as indicated in each figure legend. The Welch two-sample t test was used to compare two values.
Data availability
The datasets generated during the current study, including source data for the figures in the paper, are available in the figshare repository (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4168931)70
Additional information
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.
Doya, K. Modulators of decision making. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 410–416 (2008).
- 2.
Wang, X.-J. in Neuroeconomics 2nd edn (eds Glimcher, P. W. & Fehr, E.) Ch. 23 (Elsevier, 2014).
- 3.
Williams, C. T., Barnes, B. M., Kenagy, G. J. & Buck, C. L. Phenology of hibernation and reproduction in ground squirrels: integration of environmental cues with endogenous programming. J. Zool. 292, 112–124 (2014).
- 4.
Mori, I. & Ohshima, Y. Neural regulation of thermotaxis in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 376, 344–348 (1995).
- 5.
Hedgecock, E. M. & Russell, R. L. Normal and mutant thermotaxis in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA72, 4061–4065 (1975).
- 6.
Kuhara, A. et al. Temperature sensing by an olfactory neuron in a circuit controlling behavior of C. elegans. Science 320, 803–808 (2008).
- 7.
Kimura, K. D., Miyawaki, A., Matsumoto, K. & Mori, I. The C. elegans thermosensory neuron AFD responds to warming. Curr. Biol. 14, 1291–1295 (2004).
- 8.
Kobayashi, K. et al. Single-cell memory regulates a neural circuit for sensory behavior. Cell Rep. 14, 11–21 (2016).
- 9.
Ramot, D., MacInnis, B. L. & Goodman, M. B. Bidirectional temperature-sensing by a single thermosensory neuron in C. elegans. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 908–915 (2008).
- 10.
Clark, D. A., Biron, D., Sengupta, P. & Samuel, A. D. T. The AFD sensory neurons encode multiple functions underlying thermotactic behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. 26, 7444–7451 (2006).
- 11.
Wasserman, S. M., Beverly, M., Bell, H. W. & Sengupta, P. Regulation of response properties and operating range of the AFD thermosensory neurons by cGMP signaling. Curr. Biol. 21, 353–362 (2011).
- 12.
Inada, H. et al. Identification of guanylyl cyclases that function in thermosensory neurons of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 172, 2239–2252 (2006).
- 13.
Takeishi, A. et al. Receptor-type guanylyl cyclases confer thermosensory responses in C. elegans. Neuron 90, 235–244 (2016).
- 14.
Komatsu, H., Mori, I., Rhee, J. S., Akaike, N. & Ohshima, Y. Mutations in a cyclic nucleotide-gated channel lead to abnormal thermosensation and chemosensation in C. elegans. Neuron 17, 707–718 (1996).
- 15.
Coburn, C. M. & Bargmann, C. I. A putative cyclic nucleotide-gated channel is required for sensory development and function in C. elegans. Neuron 17, 695–706 (1996).
- 16.
Biron, D. et al. A diacylglycerol kinase modulates long-term thermotactic behavioral plasticity in C. elegans. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1499–1505 (2006).
- 17.
Barcia, G. et al. De novo gain-of-function KCNT1 channel mutations cause malignant migrating partial seizures of infancy. Nat. Genet. 44, 1255–1259 (2012).
- 18.
Heron, S. E. et al. Missense mutations in the sodium-gated potassium channel gene KCNT1 cause severe autosomal dominant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy. Nat. Genet. 44, 1188–1190 (2012).
- 19.
Wei, A. et al. Efficient isolation of targeted Caenorhabditis elegansdeletion strains using highly thermostable restriction endonucleases and PCR. Nucleic Acids Res. 30, e110 (2002).
- 20.
Yuan, A. et al. SLO-2, a K+channel with an unusual Cl-dependence. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 771–779 (2000).
- 21.
Hite, R. K. et al. Cryo-electron microscopy structure of the Slo2.2 Na+-activated K+channel. Nature 527, 198–203 (2015).
- 22.
Alqadah, A. et al. SLO BK potassium channels couple gap junctions to inhibition of calcium signaling in olfactory neuron diversification. PLoS Genet. 12, 1–28 (2016).
- 23.
Joiner, W. J. et al. Formation of intermediate-conductance calcium-activated potassium channels by interaction of Slack and Slo subunits. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 462–469 (1998).
- 24.
Kaupp, U. B. & Seifert, R. Cyclic nucleotide-gated ion channels. Physiol. Rev. 82, 769–824 (2002).
- 25.
Smith, H. K. et al. Defining specificity determinants of cGMP mediated gustatory sensory transduction in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 194, 885–901 (2013).
- 26.
Cho, S. W., Choi, K. Y. & Park, C. S. A new putative cyclic nucleotide-gated channel gene, cng-3, is critical for thermotolerance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 325, 525–531 (2004).
- 27.
Wojtyniak, M., Brear, A. G., O’Halloran, D. M. & Sengupta, P. Cell- and subunit-specific mechanisms of CNG channel ciliary trafficking and localization in C. elegans. J. Cell Sci. 126, 4381–4395 (2013).
- 28.
Yu, Y. V. et al. CaMKI-dependent regulation of sensory gene expression mediates experience – dependent plasticity in the operating range of a thermosensory neuron. Neuron 84, 919–926 (2014).
- 29.
Yuan, A. et al. The sodium-activated potassium channel is encoded by a member of the Slo gene family. Neuron 37, 765–773 (2003).
- 30.
Kim, G. E. et al. Human slack potassium channel mutations increase positive cooperativity between individual channels. Cell Rep. 9, 1661–1673 (2014).
- 31.
Milligan, C. J. et al. KCNT1 gain of function in 2 epilepsy phenotypes is reversed by quinidine. Ann. Neurol. 75, 581–590 (2014).
- 32.
Tang, Q. Y. et al. Epilepsy-related slack channel mutants lead to channel over-activity by two different mechanisms. Cell Rep. 14, 129–139 (2016).
- 33.
Salkoff, L., Butler, A., Ferreira, G., Santi, C. & Wei, A. High-conductance potassium channels of the SLO family. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 921–931 (2006).
- 34.
Santi, C. M. et al. Dissection of K+ currents in Caenorhabditis elegans muscle cells by genetics and RNA interference. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 14391–14396 (2003).
- 35.
Liu, P. et al. Genetic dissection of ion currents underlying all-or-none action potentials in C. elegans body-wall muscle cells. J. Physiol. 589, 101–117 (2011).
- 36.
Liu, P., Chen, B. & Wang, Z.-W. SLO-2 potassium channel is an important regulator of neurotransmitter release in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat. Commun. 5, 5155 (2014).
- 37.
Franceschetti, S. et al. Na+-activated K+ current contributes to postexcitatory hyperpolarization in neocortical intrinsically bursting neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 2101–2111 (2003).
- 38.
Gao, S. B. et al. Slack and Slick KNa channels are required for the depolarizing afterpotential of acutely isolated, medium diameter rat dorsal root ganglion neurons. Acta Pharmacol. Sin. 29, 899–905 (2008).
- 39.
Aoki, I., Nakano, S. & Mori, I. Oxford, in Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference 2nd edn (ed Byrne, J. H.) 415–434 (Academic Press, 2017).
- 40.
Tomioka, M. et al. The insulin/PI3-kinase pathway regulates salt chemotaxis learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuron 51, 613–625 (2006).
- 41.
Kodama, E. et al. Insulin-like signaling and the neural circuit for integrative behavior in C. elegans. Genes Dev. 20, 2955–2960 (2006).
- 42.
Chalasani, S. H. et al. Neuropeptide feedback modifies odor-evoked dynamics in Caenorhabditis elegans olfactory neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 615–621 (2010).
- 43.
Vukojevic, V. et al. A role for alpha-adducin (ADD-1) in nematode and human memory. EMBO J. 31, 1453–1466 (2012).
- 44.
Hadziselimovic, N. et al. Forgetting is regulated via musashi-mediated translational control of the Arp2/3 complex. Cell 156, 1153–1166 (2014).
- 45.
Inoue, A. et al. Forgetting in C. elegans is accelerated by neuronal communication via the TIR-1/JNK-1 pathway. Cell Rep. 3, 808–819 (2013).
- 46.
Podda, M. V. & Grassi, C. New perspectives in cyclic nucleotide-mediated functions in the CNS: the emerging role of cyclic nucleotide-gated (CNG) channels. Pflugers Arch. 466, 1241–1257 (2014).
- 47.
O’Halloran, D. M. et al. Contribution of the cyclic nucleotide gated channel subunit, CNG-3, to olfactory plasticity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Sci. Rep. 7, 169 (2017).
- 48.
Komatsu, H. et al. Functional reconstitution of a heteromeric cyclic nucleotide-gated channel of Caenorhabditis elegans in cultured cells. Brain Res. 821, 160–168 (1999).
- 49.
Bhattacharjee, A. et al. Slick (Slo2.1), a rapidly-gating sodium-activated potassium channel inhibited by ATP. J. Neurosci. 23, 11681–11691 (2003).
- 50.
Yang, B. et al. Pharmacological activation and inhibition of Slack (Slo2.2) channels. Neuropharmacology 51, 896–906 (2006).
- 51.
Bearden, D. et al. Targeted treatment of migrating partial seizures of infancy with quinidine. Ann. Neurol. 76, 457–461 (2014).
- 52.
Mikati, M. A. et al. Quinidine in the treatment of KCNT1-positive epilepsies. Ann. Neurol. 78, 995–999 (2015).
- 53.
Chong, P. F., Nakamura, R., Saitsu, H., Matsumoto, N. & Kira, R. Ineffective quinidine therapy in early onset epileptic encephalopathy with KCNT1 mutation. Ann. Neurol. 79, 502–503 (2016).
- 54.
Brenner, S. The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 77, 71–94 (1974).
- 55.
Mello, C. C., Kramer, J. M., Stinchcomb, D. & Ambros, V. Efficient gene transfer in C. elegans: extrachomosomal maintenance and integration of transforming sequences. EMBO J. 10, 3959–3970 (1991).
- 56.
Ito, H., Inada, H. & Mori, I. Quantitative analysis of thermotaxis in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. Methods 154, 45–52 (2006).
- 57.
Hillier, L. W. et al. Whole-genome sequencing and variant discovery in C. elegans. Nat. Methods 5, 183–188 (2008).
- 58.
Wicks, S. R., Yeh, R. T., Gish, W. R., Waterston, R. H. & Plasterk, R. H. Rapid gene mapping in Caenorhabditis elegans using a high density polymorphism map. Nat. Genet. 28, 160–164 (2001).
- 59.
Friedland, A. E. et al. Heritable genome editing in C. elegans via a CRISPR-Cas9 system. Nat. Methods 10, 741–743 (2013).
- 60.
Kim, H. et al. A Co-CRISPR strategy for efficient genome editing in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 197, 1069–1080 (2014).
- 61.
Frøkjær-Jensen, C., Davis, M. W., Ailion, M. & Jorgensen, E. M. Improved Mos1-mediated transgenesis in C. elegans. Nat. Methods9, 117–118 (2012).
- 62.
Frøkjær-Jensen, C. et al. Single copy insertion of transgenes in C. elegans. Nat. Genet. 40, 1375–1383 (2008).
-
- 63.
Davis, S. J., Scott, L. L., Hu, K. & Pierce-Shimomura, J. T. Conserved single residue in the BK potassium channel required for activation by alcohol and intoxication in C. elegans. J. Neurosci. 34, 9562–9573 (2014).
- 64.
Farboud, B. & Meyer, B. J. Dramatic enhancement of genome editing by CRISPR/cas9 through improved guide RNA design. Genetics 199, 959–971 (2015).
- 65.
Yoshida, A. et al. A glial K+/Cl- cotransporter modifies temperature-evoked dynamics in Caenorhabditis elegans sensory neurons. Genes Brain Behav. 15, 429–440 (2016).
- 66.
Inoue, M. et al. Rational design of a high-affinity, fast, red calcium indicator R-CaMP2. Nat. Methods 12, 64–70 (2014).
- 67.
Tian, L. et al. Imaging neural activity in worms, flies and mice with improved GCaMP calcium indicators. Nat. Methods 6, 875–881 (2009).
- 68.
KimE., SunL., GabelC. V. & Fang-YenC. Long-term imaging of Caenorhabditis elegans using nanoparticle-mediated immobilization. PLoS ONE 8, e53419 (2013).
- 69.
Tateyama, M. & Kubo, Y. Binding of Gq protein stabilizes the activated state of the muscarinic receptor type 1. Neuropharmacology 65, 173–181 (2013).
- 70.
Aoki, I. et al. SLO potassium channels antagonize premature decision making in C. elegans. (2018). https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4168931.v1
Acknowledgements
We thank T. Tamada for technical assistance on experiments; H. Matsuyama for performing Fourier transform; K. Noma and Y. You for critical reading and intensive discussion; A. Hart for valuable suggestions. Some strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (CGC), which is funded by NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440). This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP 26870266, JP 22123010, JP 16H01272, JP 16H02516, and JP 18H04693.